The Role of Kinesthesia in Pre- and Perinatal Bonding

Issue: 
Publication Date: 
03/1991
Page Count: 
18
Starting Page: 
253
Price: $10.00
Abstract: 

At birth mother and infant are in a common state of "kinesthesia." This constitutes a "kinesthetic bond." It results from the motion tracking between mother and child throughout the pregnancy. They feel each others' motion by means of touch through the uterine wall. If the sensory information they use to define their relationship is disrupted by physical separation after birth, before other sensory modes of relating are established, the relationship may suffer. When parent-infant bonding is understood in this light, movement following skills can be learned to enhance it or repair it when it has been harmed.

Kinesthetic bonding is enhanced when parents become skilled in using the components of space, timing and effort to match their movement to that of their baby. This skill is based on understanding the anatomical and functional characteristics of babies' little bodies. Bonding also benefits when the environment where babies and parents interact is organized to compensate for the difference in scale of their bodies. In this way parents and babies move and interact mutually based on their kinesthetic bond.

References: 

Ainsworth, M.D., 1973. The development of mother-infant attachment. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Barnard, K., & Eyres, S. J., (Eds.), 1979. Child health assessment, part 2: The first years of life. Wash. D.C.: U.S. Govt. Printing Office. Pub. No./d/ DHEW No. HRA 79-25.

Birdwhistle, R., 1970. Kinesics and context: essays on body motion communication. Philadelphia: University of Penn. Press.

Blechscmidt, E., 1960. The stages of Human Development before Birth. New York: S. Karger.

_____, 1977. The beginnings of human life. New York: C.V. Mosby Company.

Bowlby, J., 1969. Attachment and Loss. Vol. I, Attachment. New York: Basic Books, Inc.

_____, 1973. Attachment and Loss. Vol. II, Separation, Anxiety and Anger. New York: Basic Books Inc.

_____, 1980. Attachment and Loss. Vol. Ill, Loss, Sadness and Depression. New York: Basic Books Inc.

Brazelton, T.B., 1984. In Brown (Ed.). The many facets of touch (xv-xviii). New Jersey: Johnson and Johnson Baby Products Company.

Condon, W.S., & Sander, L.W., 1974. Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: interactional participation and language acquisition, Science 183: 99-101.

Field, T.M., 1977. Effects of early separation, interactive deficits and experimental manipulations on infant-mother face-to-face interaction, Child Development. 48: 763-771.

Gardner, Garland et al. 1989. The neonate and the environment: impact on development. Chap. 24 in Merenstein and Gardner, (eds.). Handbook of Neonatal Intensive Care. St. Louis, C.V. Mosby.

Harlow, H.F., 1961. The development of affectional patterns in infant monkeys. In Determinants of Infant Behavior. Vol. I ed. B.M. Foss. London Methuen; New York: Wiley.

Heinicke, C, & Westheimer, I., 1966. Brief Separations. New York: International Press; London: Longmans Green.

Hatch, F.W., 1973. A Behavioral cybernetic interpretation of dance and dance education. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation: University of Wisconsin, Madison.

Klaus, M.H., & Kennell, J.H., 1981. Parent-infant bonding. St. Louis: C.V. Mosby, Company.

Klaus, M.H., & Kennell, J.H., 1983. Bonding: The beginnings of parent-infant attachment. St. Louis: C.V. Mosby, Company.

Kao, H. and Smith, K.U., 1971. Social feedback: determination of social learning. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 152, 289-297.

Maratos, O., 1982. Trends in the development of imitation in early infancy. In Bever, T.G., (Ed.), Regressions in Mental Development. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence Eribsum Assoc.

Maietta, L., 1986. The effects of handling training on parent-infant interaction and infant development.

Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation: Fielding Institute.

Meltzoff, A.N., & Moore, M.K., 1977. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates, Science 198: 75-78.

Richards, M.P.M., 1974. The development of psychological communication in the first year of life. In K.J. Connolly and J.S. Bruner (Eds.), The growth of competence. London and New York: Academic Press.

Robertson, J., 1953. Some responses of young children to loss of maternal care. Nurses Times. London, 49, 382-386.

Poincare, H, 1905. Science and hypothesis. London: Walter Scott Publishing Company.

Sander, L.W. and others, 1970. Early mother-infant interaction and 24-hour patterns of activity and sleep. J.Am.Acad. Child Psychiatry 9: 103-123.

Smith, K.U., & Smith, W.M., 1962. Perception and Motion: An analysis of spacestructured behavior. Philadelphia: Saunders.

Smith, K.U., & Sturgeon, M., 1971. Effects of delayed tactile feedback. Madison, Wisconsin: Behavioral Cybernetics Laboratory.

Smith, K.U., Zwerge, C, & Smith, N.J., 1963. Sensory-feedback analysis of infant control of the behavioral environment. Perceptual and Motor Skills 16, 725-732.

Ting, T., Smith, M., & Smith, K.U., 1972. Social feedback factors in rehabilitative processes and learning. American Journal of Physical Medicine 51, 86-101.

Trevarthen, C, 1977. Descriptive analysis of infant communicative behavior. In H.R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in Mother-Infant Interaction. New York: Academic Press.

Winnicott, D.W., 1985. Primary maternal preoccupation. In Collected Papers: Through paediatrics to psycho-analysis. New York: Basic Books, Incorporated, Publishers.

Frank W. Hatch, Ph.D. and Lenny Maietta, Ph.D.

Frank Hatch and Lenny Maietta are partners with two children, Silke and Cerise. They are founders of the Institute for Cybernetic Studies in Santa Fe, New Mexico where they carry out research and develop programs that apply behavioral cybernetic research findings to relationship and skill development problems. They are co-directors of Touch In Parenting Services, a commercial program for teaching parent-infant handling skills. Address correspondence to Rt. 9, Box 86 HM, Santa Fe, NM 87505.